Sexual selection and parasites
What causes sexual ornaments and behavior ("secondary
sexual characteristics": "primary" are the gonads and reproductive
equipment themselves)?
Peacock's tail, bird song repertoires, conspicuous color patterns,
sexual displays
Conspicuous problem: Darwinian question
The two possibilities for
sexual selection are (1) male-male competition (horns etc.) or
(2) female choice. (Competition is usually among males, and choice
usually by females, because females usually invest more in offspring.)
We will focus on female choice.
FISHERIAN RUNAWAY model
- trait initially advantageous
- preference and trait become genetically correlated
(trait expressed in males, preference in females)
- Requirements: heritability of trait and preference.
trait not too expensive (there is certainly no requirement that the
trait should be expensive)
- Tests/predictions:
- negative or zero correlation between male quality and trait
- genetic correlation between trait and preference (Houde and
Endler compared guppy populations in different streams)
- high variability among populations in ornaments?
ZAHAVI's HANDICAP PRINCIPLE
The handicap principle says that
traits are not chosen despite their being costly, but
because they are
costly. "Good genes" model. Ornaments are a signal of good genes:
they must be costly, otherwise you can't rely on the honesty of the
signal.
- revealing traits: indicate male condition/quality
- conditional traits: only expressed by high-quality males
- viability ("Zahavi") traits: expressed by everyone, but
tend to kill off low-quality males
The handicap principle seems paradoxical,
but models and observation have suggested that
it actually works (in part because traits are only
expressed in males)
Problem with handicap principle: what maintains genetic variation for
"goodness"? Under normal conditions (directional selection), one
would expect that soon everyone would have "good" genes and there
would be no variability (nothing for females to choose from except
luck).
Constant mutation could lead to a mutation-selection balance,
or a variable environment could keep changing what genes are "good".
Of course (as we know), parasites represent a strong driver of
variability in the biotic environment ...
HAMILTON-ZUK hypothesis
(the Red Queen returns)
- a subset of the handicap principle
- requirements:
- females prefer resistant (showy) males
(also required by Fisher)
- correlation between quality (resistance) and trait
(not required by Fisher),
or negative correlation between parasite load and trait
- (genetic) heritable variation in resistance
(the hardest one to prove)
- Parasite load decreases host viability
(otherwise there is no point in selecting resistant males)
- (genetic) heritable variation in trait: not quite
as important, since H-Z works with a conditional trait
Evidence for H-Z: between-species
If H-Z is operating, we
expect a positivecorrelation between parasite load and
showiness across species.
This correlation has been found, for example, in Hamilton and Zuk's
original study which correlated human judgements of species brightness
with information on ectoparasite load. Other studies have been more
equivocal.
There are some problems with between-species comparisons, though:
- reverse causality: showiness could cause parasitism
(e.g. brighter species could attract more ectoparasites, or
species that invest more in showiness could have less to
invest in parasite resistance)
- ecological correlates: showiness and parasitism could
both be driven by other factors (hole-nesting, polygyny)
- falsifiability: when a correlation is not found, how do
you decide whether the data are really good enough to reject?
- phylogenetic controls
- dynamics: how do we know that parasite-driven sexual selection
couldn't be so effective that it would drive parasite loads to
lower levels in showy species?
Evidence for H-Z: within species
Guppies
- Gyrodactylus turbulli infestation lowers brightness of parasitized
males
- it also reduces the number of sexual displays
- females prefer brighter males
- we don't know whether resistance is heritable
- we can't rule out transmission avoidance (see below)
Barn swallows
- females prefer males with longer tails
(manipulative experiments, shortening and lengthening male tails)
- males with longer tails tend to have fewer mites (field
observation)
- chicks with heavy mite loads are smaller at fledging,
which leads to lower survivorship/fecundity/fitness
- heritable variation in resistance:
cross-fostering offspring of long-tailed males in the nests of
short-tailed males (and vice versa) shows that offspring of
long-tailed fathers inherit their fathers' low mite loads.
This rules out the hypotheses that
- long-tailed males have just avoided parasitism by luck
- females are selecting males for paternal care (direct benefits)
[and offspring are less parasitized, e.g., because they're better nourished]
- offspring benefit by avoiding direct transmission of
mites from fathers
Meta-analysis
- negative relationship between parasite load & expression of
secondary sex characters (more in experiments)
- no difference with/without parental care
- no difference between behavioral and morphological traits
- direct measures of immune function give stronger results
than measures of parasite load
- stronger for ectoparasites (transmission avoidance?)
Alternatives
Alternatives to H-Z: both can be tested
(with some difficulty) by manipulative cross-fostering
experiments
- selection for parental care (direct benefits)
[can be ruled out in species without parental care]
- selection for parasite avoidance.
e.g.: sage grouse & red ink "haemotomas"
[can be ruled out for non-directly transmitted parasites;
stronger evidence for ectoparasites suggests parasite
avoidance]