The Red Queen: parasitism, arms races, and the origin and maintenance of sexual reproduction

Ben Bolker

Parasitism and infectious diseases lead to ...
../pix/tenniel1.png
antagonistic coevolution between hosts and parasites, or arms races. As it turns out, this antagonistic coevolution is one potential solution to another evolutionary paradox ...

1  The cost(s) of sex

One of the things we tend to miss as a result of our own, human biology and culture is that in a perfect, homogeneous world where we were just trying to produce as many copies of our genes as possible, as quickly as possible (which is after all the name of the Darwinian game), sex would be a really bad idea. (This is one of the greatest strengths of theory in biology-making us see that there are great questions about some of the things we don't see in nature, rather than just explaining the things we do see.)
There are several theoretical problems with sex, often referred to as costs of sex.
Cost of outbreeding: too much variation could be bad, if sexual recombination breaks up co-adapted gene complexes. This would lead to what is called outbreeding depression; hybrid sterility or inviability is the extreme case of this, when an organism mates across species lines.
Cost of mating: in environments where population densities are very low, it can be hard to find a mate. Activities associated with mating can be costly (in terms of time or energy) or risky (in terms of predation or transmission of disease).
Cost of male function: why spend time, energy and nutrients producing males when you could be producing females? (Parthenogenetic females manage quite happily producing females alone.) Even if you are a hermaphrodite, it still costs more to maintain the machinery to produce both sperm and eggs than to produce eggs alone.
Cost of meiosis: why pass on only one copy of your genes when you could pass on two? This is often considered the strongest and most basic cost of sex, because it imposes a 50to asexual lineages: somehow, sexuals have to be twice as fit to make up for the cost of meiosis.
There are many variants on sexuality, ranging all the way from perfect clonal reproduction through various degrees of mixing (ploidy cycles; selfing hermaphrodites [with variants among plants like monoecy, having male and female flowers on the same plant and monocliny, having pistils and stamens within the same flower]; outcrossing hermaphrodites; and dioecy, having separate male and female organisms. It's worth thinking a bit about whether all of the aforementioned costs apply to all of these examples ... the evolution of sex is also closely tied to the evolution of recombination.
The cost of meiosis is the largest and most general of these costs, and applies generally across all sexually reproducing organisms; it implies that sexual organisms ought to have at least a twofold advantage, somehow, in order to maintain themselves. (The only counterexample would be if "all else were not equal", and sexual individuals could produce twice as many offspring as asexuals [to produce the same net number of gene copies] - but this is typically not true.) [See model in Lively 1996.]

2  Hypotheses for the maintenance of sex

So, in that case, what benefits might sex have to allow it to have started in the first place/be maintained over evolutionary time?
In all of this, we need to be careful distinguishing the true effects of sexual reproduction. Ecologists tend to assume it produces "more variable" offspring, but this is not necessarily the case. What sex really does is to allow recombination of different genotypes ... what is the true relationship between sexual reproduction and variability? It depends on population size, how frequently asexual lineages are split off from the sexual population and how, etc. etc..
How can we test these different hypotheses?
It's tricky. As with many other evolutionary questions, which are too deeply rooted to mess with experimentally (i.e. we can't make sexual and asexual versions of organisms experimentally), we have the following kinds of options:
Correlational studies: where would we expect to see each one? Reproductive assurance = low population densities; lottery model = high-variability environments; tangled bank = high-density environments; Red Queen = high-parasite environments
Observational studies: are the basic (qualitative) assumptions of the models satisfied? What about the quantitative requirements for them to operate in real life? What about corollaries/auxiliary predictions of each hypothesis?
Detailed models:
© 2005 Ben Bolker


File translated from TEX by TTH, version 3.66.
On 10 Jan 2005, 13:26.